Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Comparative risk judgements for oral health hazards among Norwegian adults: a cross sectional study

Abstract

Background

This study identified optimistic biases in health and oral health hazards, and explored whether comparative risk judgements for oral health hazards vary systematically with socio-economic characteristics and self-reported risk experience.

Methods

A simple random sample of 1,190 residents born in 1972 was drawn from the population resident in three counties of Norway. A total of 735 adults (51% women) completed postal questionnaires at home.

Results

Mean ratings of comparative risk judgements differed significantly (p < 0.001) from the mid point of the scales. T-values ranged from -13.1 and -12.1 for the perceived risk of being divorced and loosing all teeth to -8.2 and -7.8 (p < 0.001) for having gum disease and toothdecay. Multivariate analyses using General Linear Models, GLM, revealed gender differences in comparative risk judgements for gum disease, whereas social position varied systematically with risk judgements for tooth decay, gum disease and air pollution. The odds ratios for being comparatively optimistic with respect to having gum disease were 2.9, 1.9, 1.8 and 1.5 if being satisfied with dentition, having a favourable view of health situation, and having high and low involvement with health enhancing and health detrimental behaviour, respectively.

Conclusion

Optimism in comparative judgements for health and oral health hazards was evident in young Norwegian adults. When judging their comparative susceptibility for oral health hazards, they consider personal health situation and risk behaviour experience.

Peer Review reports

Background

Perceived vulnerability to disease and injury is assumed to be a motivating factor for behaviour change in a number of theoretical models [1]. Health education campaigns have focused on influencing people's risk perceptions by exposure to relevant risk information. However, there is a notion that people do not draw personal implications from risk information. This, in turn, has been related to self-enhancing processes of social comparisons or unrealistic optimism [2], the tendency to perceive negative events as less likely and positive events as more likely to self than to others [2, 3]. From a practical point of view this phenomenon could hinder the adoption of preventive behaviour and thereby undermine the effectiveness of health educational efforts. If health and oral health hazards primarily concern other people and not oneself – there might be no reason to adapt ones behaviour.

A sizeable literature has confirmed optimism in comparative risk judgements with respect to various health and safety risks, ranging from catching a cold to having AIDS and experiencing an accident [47]. People might not, however, be optimistic about all health problems and the amount of optimism varies substantially from hazard to hazard. According to social comparison theory, people would be more likely to underestimate their comparative health risk particularly if the illness /injuries are perceived to be under control or are something that they have not yet experienced [1, 2]. Adolescents estimate their risk for developing serious chronic diseases as much lower than others, but are less optimistic about more common hazards such as avoiding a flu or breaking a leg [8]. So far, studies related to comparative risk judgements for oral health hazards are almost lacking. Moreover, studies regarding optimistic biases in adult populations share the limitation of gathering data from convenient groups of generally healthy college students. It is not obvious that those findings apply to the rest of the population. Unrealistic optimism might be gender dependent and vary with health situation and other social and behavioural characteristics in the population. This study, therefore, examined perceived vulnerability to oral health hazards focusing a representative sample of young Norwegian adults.

In early research on perceived vulnerability among school-aged children, girls felt more vulnerable than boys to minor illnesses and were more likely to engage in preventive behaviours [9]. In a study by Morrongiello and Rennie [10], boys engaged in more risk behaviours than girls but were even though more optimistic about avoiding injury. On the other hand, Whalen et al [8] and Eye et al [11] did not identify any gender differences with respect to the perceived vulnerability for illness and injuries among school-aged children.

A number of studies have provided evidence consistent with the notion that people primarily use direct personal experience when inferring their susceptibility to health hazards [6]. Weinstein [1] argued that people seem able and willing to incorporate knowledge about their family history, personality and physical and psychological conditions into their risk perceptions, but do not easily recognise the relationship between their own actions and the risks they run. A study of adolescents and their parents did show that girls whose mother had cancer felt more vulnerable to cancer than their counterparts with healthy mothers [12]. It is evident from studies of children and adults, that more experience with previous risk-taking behaviour associates with higher risk appraisals for future negative events [6, 13]. A positive relationship between an increased sense of vulnerability and preventive behaviours has been demonstrated as well [6, 13]. In Norway, adult smokers take account of smoking risks when deciding whether or not to continue to smoke [14], whereas adolescents consider their vulnerability to oral health hazards when deciding whether or not to engage in preventive behaviours [15]. During the last 30 years, oral health has improved considerable among Norwegian adults, particularly among the younger cohorts [16] In 2000, the mean DMFT at age 18 was 5.1, which represents a 50% reduction since 1985 [17]. On the other hand, the national consumption of carbonated soft drinks per capita increased from 89 litres in 1992 to 118 litres in 2000 [18] and research suggest a concomitant increase at the individual level particularly in youth [19]. It seems relevant to study the proximity of perceived risks about oral health hazards in young Norwegian adults.

Purpose

The present study examined whether an optimistic bias is present in the comparative risk judgements for various health-and oral health hazards among Norwegian adults. Moreover, this study explored whether risk judgements for oral health hazards vary systematically with gender and socio-economic status and whether young adults take account of personal risk experience when considering their susceptibility to oral health hazards.

Methods

Subjects and procedure

A simple random sample of 1,190 residents born in 1972 (48% was women) was drawn by the Directorate of Taxes from a population of 13,550 persons (sampling fraction 8.8%) resident in 3 Norwegian counties on 1st January 1997. Twenty-three subjects were lost because of wrong addresses, living abroad and due to mental retardation. A mail questionnaire with an explanatory letter and a self-addressed and pre-paid envelope for the reply was posted in March 1997. To promote participation, the subjects were invited to write sender and address on the envelope, thereby entering the draw of a return voyage for two between Bergen (Norway) and Newcastle (England). Those who had not answered within 14 days received a reminder comprising a letter, questionnaire and stamped addressed envelope. Sixty-two percent of the eligible sample responded after one reminder. Of the 735 adults who replied, 360 (50.7%) was women and 58% reported 12 years of education or less. These figures deviate only slightly from the corresponding population characteristics.

Questionnaire

The postal questionnaire (additional file 1), contained questions with fixed response alternatives and graphic rating scales.

Measures

Comparative risk judgements were measured using the direct method [4], which has been employed extensively, for instance in national health surveys [20]. Respondents were asked "As compared to other people of your own age and gender, how do you think your own risk are for sometimes during your life experience; lung cancer, serious toothdecay, serious gum disease, loss of all teeth, having cancer, being divorced and experiencing serious pollution". The response categories were given as: (-3) "much lower", (-2) "lower", (-1) "some lower", (0) "same risk as others", (+1) "some higher", (+2) "higher", (+3) "much higher". For logistic regression analysis, dummy variables were constructed regarding oral health hazards (loss of all teeth, dental caries and gum disease) yielding the response categories (1) "below the risk of others" (including -3, -2 and -1), (0) "same or greater risk than others" (including 0, +1, +2 and +3).

Health enhancing and health detrimental behaviours were measured by asking how often each of the specified items (fruits, vegetables, whole wheat bread, vitamins/cod liver oil, cakes/biscuits, chips, soda, chocolate/sweets) were consumed during the past 3 months. Five-point response scales were used ranging from (1)"several times a day" to (5) seldom or never. Smoking habits were recorded as (1) "daily", (2) "sometimes" and (3) "never", whereas alcohol consumption (wine, beer, spirits) was assessed from a scale (1) "6–7 times a week to (8) "not during the last 3 months". Use of dental floss, toothpicks, toothbrush and fluoride containing mouth rinse were recorded on 7-point scales, ranging from (1) "several times a day" to (7) "never". A PCA analysis (with the 12 original measures of health and oral health related behaviours) revealed two factors. The activities included in the two factors were added into two sum scores: "health enhancing behaviour" (Mean = 30, SD 2.4, range 4–20, Cronbach's alpha = .45) and health detrimental behaviour (Mean = 15.5, SD= 2.4, range = 4–20, Cronbach's alpha .60). For a detailed description of the PCA and the construction of the two behavioural indices see [21].

Self-assessed health status was measured by one question, i.e "On a scale from 0 = no health problems to 10 = great health problems where will you fit in"? For analysis the responses were dichotomised using a cut-off point ≤ 1, where (1) "at most one problem" and (2) "more than one problem".

Satisfaction with teeth was assessed by one question in terms of "How satisfied are you with your teeth as they are today" The responses ranged from (1) "very satisfied" to (5) "very dissatisfied". A dummy variable was constructed for logistic regression analysis (1) "satisfied" (2) "dissatisfied"

Gender was coded as (1) "male" and (2) "female".

Social position/social class – Two measures were used, one relating to occupational prestige and income and one relating to level of education. The measure for occupational status was derived by coding job and employment descriptions according to the classification of the Central Bureau of Statistics, grouping occupations mainly according to training and qualifications needed for the job. The codes were finally converted into three categories (1) "manual worker" (fisherman, farmer, semiskilled and unskilled manual workers, driver) (2) "non-manual worker" (teachers, self-employed, health worker, manager, trades people and (3) "full time university students". The second measure of social class was based on years of schooling. The highest qualification received, when leaving school was used to construct a four-point educational status score. Educational status was categorised as (1) ≤ 12 years (i.e. lower level, including those who had left school at age 15 and 18 and were without further education) and (2) > 12 years (i.e. higher level including the holders of technical trade, diplomas and higher degrees).

Statistical analyses

Univariate analyses were performed using cross-tabulation and Chi square statistics. Optimistic bias in comparative risk assessments for health and oral health hazards was assessed by use of one-sample t-statistics. Multivariate analyses were performed using General Linear Models (GLM), and logistic regression analysis. For all analyses, a two-sided significance level of 5% was applied.

Results

Table 1 One sample t-statistics of comparative risk judgements for health and oral health hazards among young adults.

One sample t-statistics to indicate whether the mean ratings of the questions assessing comparative risk judgements were different from the mid-point of the scales (i.e. indicating an optimistic bias) provided significant effects (p < 0.001) for the eight comparative risks judgements investigated. As shown in table 1, t-values ranged from t = -13.1 and t = -12.1 (p < 0.001) for being divorced and loosing all teeth to t = -1.7 and t = -1.8 (p > 0.05) for experiencing cancer and pollution, respectively. Multivariate analysis of variance (GLM) with the eight comparative risk judgements revealed a significant multivariate effect, F = 43.34 p < 0.000. Significant effects occurred for the risk judgements of having lung cancer F = 76.6 (p < 0.001), gum disease F = 60.7 (p < 0.001), toothdecay F = 63.6 (p < 0.001) and tooth loss F = 143.4 (p < 0.001). In other words, 36.1% of the respondents reported their comparative risk of having lung cancer to be below average and 19.8% reported their risk to be above average. The corresponding figures regarding gum disease were 29.1% and 14.6%, regarding tooth-decay 38.7% and 19.5% and regarding tooth loss 33.9% and 9.7%.

A GLM with educational level, social position and gender as fixed factors and the eight comparative risk judgements as dependent variables, controlling for all two-way interactions revealed multivariate main effects of gender F= 2.049 p < 0.05, social position F = 1.749, p < 0.05 and educational level F= 2.890, p < 0.05. Estimated marginal means revealed that women gave significantly lower comparative risk judgement of gum disease than did men (-.39 versus -.19, p < 0.05). Social position varied systematically with perceived risk of having gum disease (F = 3.47, p < 0.05), tooth decay (F = 5.58, p < 0.05) and experiencing pollution (F = 3.1, p < 0.05). The estimated marginal means regarding gum disease amounted to -.27, -.17 and -.45 for manual workers, non-manual workers and full time university students, respectively. The corresponding figures regarding comparative risk judgements for toothdecay were -.17, -.43 and -.59. Educational level varied systematically only with comparative risk judgements for experiencing an accident (F = 13.1, p < 0.000). The mean comparative risk judgements amounted to -.46 and -.11 among lower and higher educated adults, respectively.

Table 2 depicts Pearson's correlation coefficients among comparative risk judgements for gum disease, toothdecay and tooth loss and personal risk experience in terms of self-reported health enhancing behaviour, health detrimental behaviour, health status and satisfaction with teeth. Preason's r varied form -.17 (p < 0.001) to .48 (p < 0.001).

Table 2 Pearson's correlations among health enhancing behaviour, health detrimental behaviour, perceived number of health complaints and satisfaction with teeth and perceived comparative risk judgements for gum disease, tooth decay and loosing all teeth.

Table 3,4,5 depict the estimated multivariate models, adjusted odds ratio and 95% CI of comparative risk judgements for gum disease, toothdecay and tooth loss, applied as binary outcome variables (1= below the risk of others, 0= as big or bigger risk than others). The odds ratio for being comparatively optimistic with respect to the perceived risk of having gum disease were 2.9, 1.9, 1.8 and 1.5 if being satisfied with oral health status, satisfied with own health status, if having high involvement in health enhancing and low involvement in health detrimental behaviour, respectively. The odds ratio for being unrealistic optimistic about having toothdecay were 4.7, 1.6 and 1.5 if being satisfied with oral health, viewing health situation favourably and engaging in less health detrimental behaviour, respectively. As compared to being a full-time university student, manuals were less likely of being unrealistically optimistic about having toothdecay. The only statistical significant predictors of comparative risk judgements of having tooth loss were perceived oral health status and perceived health status with odds ratios of 3.2 and 1.8, respectively. Significant second order effects in terms of regression coefficients (B) were identified for the terms gender by health detrimental behaviour (B = 0.70, p < 0.05) and gender by health enhancing behaviour (-0.85, p < 0.05) on comparative risk judgements for having tooth loss and toothdecay, respectively. Odds ratios for being comparatively optimistic about toothdecay if engaged in health enhancing behaviour was 2.4 (95% CI, 1.5–3.9) for men and 1.1 (95% CI 0.8–1.8) for women. Correspondingly, the risk of being unrealistic optimistic about tooth loss, if involved with health detrimental behaviour, was 0.5 (95% CI 0.5–1.3) and 0.4 (95% CI 0.2–0.7) among men and women, respectively.

Table 3 Logistic regression in terms of odds ratios, OR and 95% Confidence Interval, CI, for young adults' comparative risk judgements for gum disease (1 = below the risk of others, 0 = the same risk as others and above) according to gender, social position and various aspects of personal risk experience
Table 4 Logistic regression in terms of odds ratios, OR and 95% Confidence Interval, CI, for young adults' comparative risk judgements for toothdecay (1 = below the risk of others, 0 = the same risk as others and above) according to social position and various aspects of personal risk experience
Table 5 Logistic regression in terms of odds ratios, OR and 95% Confidence Interval, CI, for young adults' comparative risk judgements for tooth loss (1 = below the risk of others, 0 = the same risk as others and above) according to various aspects of personal risk experience

Discussion

When assessed in comparative terms, the Norwegian adults, as a group, claimed, they were less at risk than similar others across various health-and oral health hazards, except for the comparative risk judgements of experiencing cancer and pollution. Hence, the unrealistic optimism effect, initially documented by Weinstein [14], in his studies of US-college students was replicated, with health and oral health hazards in a representative sample of young Norwegian adults. The hazards which adults thought they had most chance of experiencing in comparison to their peers were cancer and pollution. Absence of unrealistic optimism in comparative risk judgements for cancer has been documented elsewhere [2]. Kreuter and Stercher [22] and Whalen et al [8] compared cancer to several other health and environmental hazards (heart attack, stroke and motor vehicle crash) and found that the perceived risk of having cancer was much greater than for the other hazards investigated. Young adults might perceive their risk of having cancer and experiencing air pollution as uncontrollable and thus have greater fear of diseases and environmental hazards they do not know much about. On the other hand, the subjects investigated in this study were most unrealistically optimistic about their chances of experiencing a divorce and loosing all their teeth.

A sizeable amount of literature has reported on optimistic biases in comparative risk assessments across different age, sex and cultures and across a variety of health, safety and environmental risks [see [23]]. In addition, there is evidence that perceived invulnerability occurs not only in relatively immune people but also among groups considered at high risk [8]. This study adds to existing evidence by indicating the presence of optimistic biases in comparative risk judgements for various oral health hazards among young adults from the general population. The existence of an optimistic bias might be true as long as the individuals who provided personal risk estimates are considered fairly representative of the comparison group that they use. It remains unclear, however, whether this evidence reflects any underestimation of personal risk in absolute or true terms on the part of Norwegian adults at age 25. A reduction in dental caries experience as well as in the prevalence of periodontal diseases has been observed among adults in Norway [24]. It is likely that the consistently low vulnerability observed in this study may be attributed to the fact that young people have little personal experience with those hazards enlisted that emerge later in life. This accords with Weinstein's [1] notion that conditions most likely to elicit unrealistic optimism are those associated with the often, incorrect belief that if the problem has not yet appeared it is unlikely to occur in the future. This evidence is however still equivocal and other studies have shown that ill and healthy people do not differ significantly in their unrealistic optimism scores [25].

The present finding which showed generally modest differences in the levels of optimism with respect to gender and social position are consistent with what has been reported previously [24]. Nevertheless, females felt more optimistic about their chances of getting gum disease than did their male counterparts. Mc Kenna et al. [26] reported a similar gradient in the results from a smoking population. Moreover, manual workers were less unrealistically optimistic about their chances of having toothdecay and gum disease than were university students. This probably reflects the statistics showing that in the overall young adult population lower socio-economic status groups are more at risk for oral diseases than their higher socio-economic counterparts [24]. A social comparison model would suggest that university students and girls report lower perceived risk due to lack of health knowledge and personal experience. Finally, females and university students might be relatively accurate in their perceived vulnerability if they engage in more health enhancing and less health detrimental behaviours than do their manual worker-and male counterparts. A number of studies have demonstrated that individuals who report higher standards of education and income are more likely to engage in preventive oral hygiene behaviours, less smoking and have better eating habits [27, 28]. Compared to males, females are generally less likely to smoke, consume less alcohol, pay more attention to their diet and engage in more preventive oral health behaviour [29].

Consistent with previous findings in other health related domains [6], and at odds with others [25], Norwegian adults seem to consider personal risk experience when evaluating their susceptibility for oral health hazards. As shown in Table 3,4,5, adults who rated their health and oral health favourably, engaged in more health enhancing – and less health detrimental behaviour, were more likely to be optimistically biased regarding gum disease, toothdecay and tooth loss than were their counterparts in the opposite groups. As far as oral health threatening behaviours are concerned, the biggest risk takers in the sample of Norwegian adults were those least likely to exaggerate their own invulnerability, an outcome not predicted by the invulnerability hypothesis. This systematic variation with reported standing on actual risk factors, indicates that at least in a relative sense adults' personal risk perceptions were fairly accurate. Moreover, interaction effects suggest that when evaluating their comparative susceptibility for oral health hazards, females were more likely than males to consider oral health threatening activities. The importance of individual differences has been demonstrated previously [30] for instance in that personality style interacted with behavioural risk in predicting high school students' AIDS risk perceptions.

These results demonstrating the presence of unrealistic optimism have obvious practical implications. Providing young adults with vicarious experience in terms of risk information about their own age group or people in general, might not lead them to accept this information as relevant to themselves. A more promising approach might be to provide self-relevant information, encouraging people to recognise their own vulnerability. Making the health and oral health risk information personalised (e.g. reviewing the family medical history, socio-economic differences in disease incidence) would be more likely to alter young adults' sense of risk than more conventional health education approaches. Identification of additional factors that influence perceived risk for oral health hazards appears to be an avenue for future research.

References

  1. Weinstein ND: Perceptions of personal susceptibility to harm. In Primary prevention of AIDS: psychological approaches. Edited by: VM Mays, GW Albee, SF Schneider. 1989, Newbury Park: Sage, 142-167.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Klein WM, Weinstein ND: Social comparison and unrealistic optimism about personal risk. In: Health, Coping and Well-being: Perspectives from Social Comparison theory. Edited by: BP Buunk, FX Gibbons. 1997, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 25-61.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Weinstein ND: Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems: conclusions from a community-wide sample. J Behav Med. 1987, 10: 481-501.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Weinsten ND: Unrealistic optimism about future life events. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980, 30: 806-20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Hoorens V: Unrealistic optimism in health and safety risks. In: Social Psychology and health: European perspectives. Aldershot: Avebury. Edited by: DT Rutter, L Quine. 153-174.

  6. van der Pligt J: Risk perception and self protective behavior. Eur Psychol. 1996, 1: 34-43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Schwarzer R: Optimism, vulnerability and self-beliefs as health related cognitions: a systematic overview. Psychol and Health. 1994, 6: 23-38.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Whalen CK, Henker B, O'Neil R, Hollingshead J, Holman A, Moore B: Optimism in children's judgements of health and environmental risks. Health Psychol. 1994, 13: 319-25. 10.1037//0278-6133.13.4.319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Gochman DS: Development of health beliefs. Psychol Rep. 1972, 31: 2509-66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Morrongiello BA, Rennie H: Why do boys engage in more risk taking than girls? The role of attributions, beliefs and risk appraisals. J Pediatr Psychol. 1998, 23: 33-43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Ey S, Klesges LM, Patterson SM, Hadley W, Barnard M, Alpert BS: Racial differences in adolescents' perceived vulnerability to disease and injury. J Behav Med. 2000, 23: 421-35. 10.1023/A:1005568930849.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Ponder M, Lee J, Green J, Richards M: Family history and perceived vulnerability to some common diseases: A study of young people and their parents. J Med Genet. 1996, 33: 485-92.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Otten W, van der Pligt J: Risk and behavior. The mediating role of risk appraisal. Acta Psychol. 1992, 80: 325-46. 10.1016/0001-6918(92)90054-H.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Rise J, Strype J, Sutton S: Comparative risk ratings and lung cancer among Norwegian smokers. Addiction Research and Theory. 2001

    Google Scholar 

  15. Åstrøm AN, Rise J: Analysis of adolescents' beliefs about the outcome of using dental floss and drinking non-sugared mineral water. Community Dent Oral Epidemiol. 1996, 24: 211-216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Schuller AA: Changes in oral health in adults. Trøndelag, Norway 1973–1983–1994. Thesis. Oslo: University of Oslo. 1999

    Google Scholar 

  17. helsetilsyn Statens: Tanhelset-jenesten i Norge. Årsmelding. 2000–1999. Oslo: Statens helsetilsyn;. 2001

    Google Scholar 

  18. Bryggeri of mineralvannforeningen: Tall og fakta om øl, brus of vann i Norge. Oslo;. 2001, 12-

    Google Scholar 

  19. Åstrøm AN, Samdal O: Time trends in oral health behaviours among Norwegian adolescents: 1985–97. Acta Odontol Scand. 2001, 59: 193-200. 10.1080/00016350152509193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Sutton SR: How ordinary people in Great Britain perceive the health risks of smoking. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1998, 52: 338-39.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Åstrøm AN, Rise J: Socio-economic differences in patterns of health and oral health behaviour in 25 year old Norwegians. Cin Oral Invest. 2001, 5: 122-28. 10.1007/s007840000102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Kreuter M, Strecher V: Changing inaccurate perceptions of health risks: results from a randomized trial. Health Psychol. 1995, 14: 56-63. 10.1037//0278-6133.14.1.56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Åstrøm AN, Awadia AK, Bjorvatn K: Perceptions of susceptibility to oral health hazards: a study of women in different cultures. Community Dent Oral Epidemiol. 1999, 27: 268-74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Schuller AA, Holst D: Changes in the oral health of adults from Trøndelag, Norway 1973–1983–1994. Community Dent Oral Epidemiol. 1998, 26: 201-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Cohn LD, Schydlower M, Foley J, Copeland R: Adolescent misinterpretation of health risk expressions. Pediatrics. 1995, 95: 713-16.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. McKenna FP, Warburton DM, Winwood M: Exploring the limits of optimism: the case of smoker's decision making. British Journal of Psychology. 1993, 84: 389-94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Payne BJ, Locker D: Relationship between dental and general health behaviors in a Canadian population. J Public Health Dent. 1996, 56: 198-204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Rosin P, Peters J, Robling MR: Social class and preventive behaviour: a British example. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1995, 49: 28-32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Sakki TK, Knuutila MLE, Anttila SS: Lifestyle, gender and occupational status as determinants of dental health behaviour. J Clin Periodontol. 1998, 25: 566-70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Gladis MM, Michela JL, Walter HJ, Vaughan JD: High school students' perception of AIDS risk: Realistic appraisal or motivated denial?. Health Psychol. 1992, 11: 307-16. 10.1037//0278-6133.11.5.307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the young people who completed the questionnaire and Ms Linda K. Forshaw for clerical assistance. The financial support from NDD's Research Fund, the Swedish Patent Revenue research Fund and the University of Bergen is gratefully acknowledged. We are grateful to Color Line, Bergen for donating the return trip for two used to encourage participation

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anne Nordrehaug Åstrøm.

Additional information

Competing interests

None declared.

Electronic supplementary material

12903_2002_5_MOESM1_ESM.doc

Additional file 1: Health and health behaviour among 25 year old Norwegian adults Cross sectional study and self-reported data. (DOC 22 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Åstrøm, A.N. Comparative risk judgements for oral health hazards among Norwegian adults: a cross sectional study. BMC Oral Health 2, 3 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6831-2-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6831-2-3

Keywords