Skip to main content

Memory decline in older individuals predicts an objective indicator of oral health: findings from the Sydney Memory and Ageing Study

Abstract

Background

Growing evidence suggests that there is an association between poor oral health and cognitive function in late adulthood. However, most studies to date have relied on cross-sectional research methods that do not permit inferences about the temporality of any association. Moreover, the few longitudinal studies that do exist have typically relied on small samples and quite limited cognitive or oral health assessments. The aim of the present study was therefore designed to provide the first direct evaluation of whether cognitive function is predictive of poor oral health in older adults.

Methods

This longitudinal research included data from 339 participants aged 70 years or older from The Sydney Memory and Ageing Study (MAS), a large cohort of healthy community-dwelling older adults. Cognitive function was assessed using a battery of tests at baseline (Wave 1) in 2005 and six years later (Wave 4) in 2011. In 2015 (Wave 6), participants were assessed for oral health using the Oral Health Assessment Tool (OHAT), number of functional occluding pairs of natural teeth and sublingual resting saliva pH (SRSpH). Ordinal least squares regression analysis was used to model the effect of cognitive function on total OHAT score, and binomial logistic regression used for SRSpH and occluding pairs of functional teeth.

Results

Two models were tested. In the partially adjusted model, age, gender and years of education were included. The fully adjusted model additionally included medical conditions, general health, depression, smoking, alcohol consumption, functionality, and dental care utilization. The key finding to emerge was that a six-year change in memory (from Wave 1 to Wave 4) was associated with lower sublingual resting saliva pH at Wave 6 in partially (Odds Ratio (OR) = 0.65) and fully adjusted model (OR = 0.63).

Conclusions

This longitudinal study provides further evidence that a relationship between cognitive function and oral health exists, and also points to this relationship potentially being bi-directional, as previous evidence suggests. The findings from the study also suggest that older adults who present with greater than normal memory decline at an earlier point in life were more likely to experience poor oral health when this was evaluated at a later time-point, four years later.

Peer Review reports

Background

The proportion of older adults worldwide is increasing and projected to continue to do so. In 2019, the number of people globally aged 65 or over was estimated to be 703 million, and this is expected to double to 1.5 billion by 2050 [1]. This change in the ageing demographic is important for several reasons, not least because older age remains the single most important predictor of cognitive impairment, which is the leading cause of dependence and disability among elderly adults [2]. Dementia, which is the pathological extremity of cognitive impairment affects more than 50 million people worldwide, and is a major public health burden [3].

The importance of oral health is often not fully appreciated by health care professionals working in aged care, yet oral health is fundamental to older adults’ general health, overall well-being and quality of life [4]. Poor oral health directly impacts older adults’ physical wellbeing, by affecting their ability to eat and swallow properly, contributing to weight loss, dehydration, malnutrition, and frailty [5], and can also negatively impact social communication [6]. Periodontal disease severity and Porphyromonas gingivalis have also been significantly associated with all-cause mortality in cardiovascular disease and ischemic stroke [7]. Further, both Porphyromonas gingivalis and its proteases, involved in periodontal disease, have been identified in the brains of Alzheimer’s patients and have been identified as one of many potential risk factors for the development of this disorder [8].

Relative to younger adults, older people are more vulnerable to a range of oral health problems [9]. A range of potential risk factors have been studied, including reduced functional capacity, decreased physical strength, the presence of comorbid medical conditions, polypharmacy and of particular interest in recent years, cognitive impairment [10, 11]. A recent systematic review concluded that there was moderate to good evidence for an association between three specific cognitive domains (learning and memory, complex attention and executive function) and older adults’ oral health [12]. However, this review also highlighted an important limitation of this literature, noting that most studies conducted to date that have tested this relationship have relied on cross-sectional research methods. Such methods do not allow inferences to be made about the temporality of any association and, consequently, causality.

Interestingly, both potential directions of causality appear viable. This is because, on the one hand, it has been suggested that poorer oral health may contribute to cognitive decline via specific biological mechanisms such as common inflammatory processes linked to periodontitis [13] or reduced masticatory performance leading to reduced nutritional intake [14]. With respect to the reverse, it has been suggested that cognitive decline might lead to oral health deterioration as functionality in daily life decreases, via reduced attention to oral hygiene or inadequate use of dental health services.

However, surprisingly few studies have used longitudinal research methods to directly test whether there is evidence to support this latter causal pathway. Although it has been shown that older adults with poorer cognitive functioning are less likely to access dental care [15], and engage in less frequent tooth brushing and related oral hygiene practices [16, 17], only six studies have directly tested whether cognitive function at an earlier time point predicts oral health at a later one [18,19,20,21,22,23]. Moreover, inferences from these studies are limited by the use of small samples [19, 21], and in five of the studies, reliance on only brief cognitive screens or medical records to quantify cognitive function [18,19,20,21,22]. While the sixth study included a large community cohort, and excellent characterisation of cognitive function [23], a limitation here was that oral health was indexed solely via self-report. Extensive literature shows that self-report measures of oral health are less accurate than more objective indices [24, 25].

Thus, although Kang et al. [23] found that lower cognitive function at baseline was associated with poorer oral health at follow up, the next important step in this literature is to establish these associations using objective indicators of oral health. The present study was therefore designed to provide the first direct evaluation of whether cognitive function is predictive of poor oral health in older adults, using a battery of validated, objective assessments to index both cognitive function and oral health. The key predictions were that older adults’ cognitive function at Wave 4, and degree of subsequent cognitive decline (from Wave 1 to Wave 4), would be predictive of their subsequent oral health, even after adjusting for important potential covariates.

Methods

Study population

Participants were drawn from the community-based Sydney Memory and Aging (MAS) study. The MAS is an ongoing, longitudinal study that originally recruited 1037 healthy individuals aged 70–90 years [26]. MAS participants have completed an extensive range of physical, cognitive and psychological assessments; at study entry commencing in 2005 (Wave 1), and follow-up assessments approximately every 2 years thereafter. To be eligible for inclusion into MAS, participants had to be free from any major neurological or psychiatric illness including a self-reported history of dementia, and also pass a cognitive screen to rule out dementia at study entry. Participants with a Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) score of < 24 adjusted for age, education and non-English speaking background were excluded.

In the present study, analyses were restricted to participants who completed an oral health assessment ten years after baseline at Wave 6 (2015) and had not been diagnosed with dementia at any time point up to Wave 4 (2011). At Wave 4, the total sample included 708 participants. Of these, 460 participants then went on to complete Wave 6, of whom 350 undertook the oral health assessments. After excluding the 11 participants who were diagnosed with dementia prior to Wave 4, the final sample consisted of 339 participants (Fig. 1). Cognitive data in the study was obtained from Wave 1 (2005) and Wave 4 (2011) respectively, and oral health data available only at Wave 6 (2015).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flowchart for participant selection for this study

Oral health

The well validated Oral Health Assessment Tool (OHAT) [27] was used for assessing oral health at Wave 6. The OHAT assesses the following aspects of oral health: (i) lips; (ii) tongue; (iii) gums and oral tissues; (iv) saliva; (v) natural teeth; (vi) dentures; (vii) oral cleanliness; and (viii) dental pain. To administer OHAT, trained research assistants inspected the oral cavity for 5–10 min. Scores on individual categories of OHAT range from healthy (0), changes from normal (1) to unhealthy (2). Sum of scores from each category was derived, giving a total score ranging from 0 (healthy) to 16 (extensive oral health problems), with higher OHAT scores indicative of poorer oral health.

Other oral health measures recorded were sublingual resting saliva pH (SRSpH) and the number of functional occluding pairs of natural teeth. Saliva was obtained from beneath participants’ tongue using a micro brush for two seconds, and rubbed on one square of 6–8 pH paper and one square of 3–5.5 pH paper. All the included participants were instructed not to eat or drink 1 h prior to the saliva test, and any related abnormalities thought to be associated with salivary pH reduction not excluded. The test paper was observed for whether the micro brush could wet the pH paper and for colour change and salivary pH levels recorded. A lower pH score was recorded in cases where the level appeared to be between two values [28]. All numbers equal to 6.6 or above were considered normal, while values lower than 6.6 were considered as greater risk for oral disease [29]. Healthy salivary pH should measure no lower than 6.6, and lower salivary pH is well-established as a risk factor for dental caries and dental erosion [30]. The number of occluding pairs (maximum of 16 pairs) was recorded by asking the participant to bite in maximum intercuspation, which was then counted objectively. This was categorized into (i) > 10 pairs, and (ii) ≤ 10 pairs of functional teeth [31].

Cognitive function

A battery of standardized cognitive assessments was used to assess cognitive function at waves 1 to 4 (neuropsychological battery was available only for waves 1–4 but not in other waves). Memory was measured using Logical Memory Story A (delayed) [32], Rey Auditory Visual Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) [33] (total learning; trials 1–5; short-term recall: trial 6, and long-term recall: trial 7) and the Benton Visual Retention Test (BVRT) [34]. Executive function was assessed using Phonemic Fluency (FAS) [35] and the Trail Making Test B [33]. To assess attention/processing speed, Digit Symbol Coding [32] and Trail Making Test A [33] were used. Animal naming [36] and the 30-item Boston Naming Test [37] were used to assess Language. Visuospatial ability was measured using the Block Design test [38]. Raw test scores were transformed to z-scores using the baseline mean and standard deviation (SD) values of a healthy reference subsample (n = 723 MAS participants) [39]. Domain scores were calculated by averaging the z-scores of component tests (except for visuo-spatial which was represented by a single test), again standardized by transforming these scores using the healthy group baseline mean and SD values. Global cognition scores were calculated by averaging individual cognitive domain scores (attention/processing speed, memory, language, executive function and visuo-spatial) and standardized against the healthy group.

Covariates

Established or suspected factors associated with dental status and cognitive function were selected based on prior literature. Socio-demographic characteristics included age, gender and years of education. Medical conditions were defined as history of stroke, diabetes, hypertension, heart disease (coronary heart disease, atrial fibrillation) [40] and lung disease (chronic obstructive pulmonary disease) up to Wave 4. Self-rated general health status was assessed on a Likert scale with anchors 1 = poor to 5 = excellent. The shorter version of Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS), consisting of 15 questions, was used to evaluate depressive symptoms [41]. Of the 15 items, 10 indicated the presence of depression when answered positively, while the rest (question numbers 1, 5, 7, 11, 13) indicated depression when answered negatively. The total score was calculated by totaling 1 point counted for each depression answer. The scores on the GDS can range from 0–15. Health related behaviours included smoking patterns (responses categorized into never, past and current smokers) and alcohol consumption (categorized as– abstainer (0 drinks), <  = 1 standard drink/day, or > 1 standard drink/day – with 1 standard drink = 10 g alcohol) [40]. Functional impairment was defined as informant-reported limitations on Bayer activities of daily living (B-ADL) [42] and basic activities of daily living (ADL) [43]. The basic ADL was assessed using the Lawton & Brody Physical Self-Maintenance Scale (PSMS) [43]. The PSMS is a scale containing 6 items of self-care with a 5-point rating scale (responses ranging from total independence to total dependence). For each item the response describing the person’s highest level of functioning is recorded (either 1 or 0). The total score on the basic ADL ranges from 0 to 6. The instrumental ADL was assessed using the Bayer-Activities of Daily Living Scale (B-ADL) [42]. The informant rates a person’s ability to perform an activity on a scale of 1 to 10 for each of the 25 items, where 1 indicates ‘never’ and 10 indicates ‘always’ have difficulty. In instance where an activity is not appropriate, not relevant, or unknown, the informant rates ‘not applicable’ or ‘unknown’. Individual item scores are then summed up. Items rated ‘not applicable’ and ‘unknown’ are not used for the computation of the total scores. The total is then divided by the number of items rates between ‘1’ and ‘10’. The total scores on B-ADL range between 1.00 and 10.00. Dental care utilization, measured at Wave 6, was based on a question asking when participant last saw a dentist (where 1 =  < 12 months ago, 2 = 1–2 years ago, 3 = 3–4 years ago, 4 = 5–6 years ago, and 5 =  > 6 years ago).

Statistical analysis

All statistical tests were 2-sided, and significance was set at p ≤ 0.05. Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistical software for Windows version 27.0. Ordinal least squares regression was used for modelling the effect of cognitive function on total OHAT score. Binomial logistic regression was used for the dichotomized categorical variables (SRSpH and pairs of functional teeth). Two sets of models were tested. In the partially adjusted model, age, gender and years of education were included. The fully adjusted model included covariates in the partially adjusted model plus medical conditions, general health, depression, smoking, alcohol consumption, functionality, and dental care utilization. Multiple imputations by chained equation were used to impute missing covariate data. Twenty imputed datasets were created and analysed, and results were pooled based on Rubin’s rules. The effects of cognitive function at Wave 4 and change in cognitive function from Wave 1 to Wave 4 were examined in separate sets of models. To capture the change in cognition, change scores were computed by subtracting Wave 1 cognitive scores from Wave 4 cognitive scores.

Results

Participant characteristics

Table 1 summarizes detailed characteristics of all participants from the MAS study. The age of the participants at study entry ranged from 70–87 years, and females were slightly over-represented (55%). The participants selected were relatively younger, more likely to be female, less likely to have coronary artery disease, had fewer depressive symptoms, had better self-rated general health and basic ADL scores, and were more likely to be non-smokers, relative to the unselected participants. In addition, of the participants selected, 303 (89%) reported their perceived general health as being good to excellent. The most prevalent condition reported in the medical history was hypertension (59%). About half of the included participants were ex-smokers (47%) and consumed alcohol (50%) on a daily/regular basis. Over a median interval of six years between Wave 1 and Wave 4, participants experienced a decline in cognitive function. The magnitude of the effect/change for the cognitive tests ranged from small (for assessments of memory) to medium (for global cognition) (see Additional file 1).

Table 1 Baseline characteristics of participants in the whole MAS sample (N = 1037), selected sample (N = 339) and unselected sample (N = 698)

Oral health characteristics

Scores on individual distribution categories for OHAT is reported in Table 2. The average OHAT score was 2.5 (range = 0–11), and mean SRSpH ranged from 4.5 to 7.8 (M = 6.1, SD = 0.5). More than half of the participants had 20 or more occluding pairs of teeth (62%), and had visited the dentist in the last 12 months (68%). Taken together, the relatively low scores on OHAT, the relatively high number of functional pairs of teeth, and frequent dentist visits indicate that majority of participants in this study were generally reporting relatively good levels of oral health (Table 3).

Table 2 Distribution of scores for individual categories of the Oral Health Assessment Tool (OHAT) for participants at Wave 6 (N = 339)
Table 3 Distribution of oral health conditions and dentist visits for participants at Wave 6 (N = 339)

Cognitive function and oral health

Oral health assessment tool (OHAT)

Lower scores in the specific cognitive domain of attention/processing speed at Wave 4, and for change in cognitive function (between Wave 1 and Wave 4) were negatively associated with OHAT scores at Wave 6 (ps = 0.028 and 0.024, respectively). This means that older adults with poorer attention/processing speed and who showed greater decline in this cognitive capacity over a six-year period (from Wave 1 to Wave 4) had more oral health problems four years later (Wave 6), after accounting for age, gender, and education. This association also remained robust when adjusted for comorbidities (see Additional file 1). However, after full adjustment for covariates neither of these associations remained significant. For the other four cognitive domains, as well as the composite global cognition measure, none of the models was significant after either partial or full adjustments for covariates were applied. These data are presented in Table 4.

Table 4 Ordinary least squares regression estimating effect of cognitive function on Oral Health Assessment Tool (OHAT) score

Sub-lingual resting saliva pH (SRSpH)

For SRSpH, in both the partially (OR 0.65, 95% CI: 0.42–1.00) and fully adjusted models (OR 0.63, 95% CI: 0.40–0.99), greater decline in memory function over the six-year period between Wave 1 and Wave 4 was predictive of lower SRSpH four years later at Wave 6 (ps = 0.050 and 0.046, respectively). This indicated that participants who experienced greater decline in memory during an earlier time point were susceptible to greater risk of acidic saliva four years later. No other significant associations emerged between cognitive function and SRSpH. These data are presented in Table 5.

Table 5 Binary logistic regression estimating effect of cognitive function on sub-lingual resting saliva pH (SRSpH)

Functional occluding pairs of natural teeth

No associations were found with functional pairs of teeth, either when looking at Wave 4 cognitive function or for degree of decline (from Wave 1 to Wave 4), between global cognition or any of the specific cognitive domains in either the partially or fully adjusted models (Table 6).

Table 6 Binomial logistic regression estimating effect of cognitive function on functional pairs of teeth

Discussion

The present study examined whether cognitive function prospectively predicted oral health outcomes in a cohort of older Australian adults. The key findings to emerge were older adults with declining memory function at an earlier point in life were then also found to be more likely to experience poor oral health when this was evaluated at a later time-point, four years later. Additionally, a six-year decline in memory function was a significant predictor of lower sublingual resting salivary pH levels four years later, with this association remaining robust in both the partially and fully adjusted models. However, no other relationships were identified between the other cognitive measures and oral health (OHAT & functional pairs of teeth).

Despite extensive literature focused on the relationship between oral health and cognitive function (PubMed identifies almost 300 hits to the search terms “oral health” AND “cognition”), and growing evidence showing that such a relationship exists, surprisingly little is understood about the temporality of this association. In particular, as noted previously, only six studies to date have used longitudinal research methods to test whether older adults’ poorer cognitive function at an earlier time point is predictive of their oral health at a later one. Moreover, in the only study to date to conduct such an assessment that included both a large sample size and an adequate assessment of cognitive function, oral health was indexed solely via self-report [23]. Thus, although Kang et al.’s [23] study provided the most compelling evidence to date that cognitive decline is predictive of later oral health deterioration, given the limitations inherent to self-report methods of oral health [24, 25], the current study adds meaningfully to the current literature by additionally including comprehensive objective and quantifiable indicators of oral health status.

The results partially align with the findings from Kang et al. [23] study, in showing that in a group of healthy, community-dwelling older adults, greater deterioration in memory function at an earlier timepoint was associated with an indicator of poorer oral health four years later. Using data from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA), Kang et al. found that poorer cognitive function at baseline was predictive of self-reported tooth loss approximately 12 years later [23]. However, our findings also extend these findings in a meaningful way. This is because our data suggest that this relationship between cognition decline and later oral health may be particularly driven by decline in specific cognitive domain(s).

While a methodological strength of Kang et al.’s study was the use of assessments from three domains known to be particularly sensitive to age-related cognitive decline (memory, executive control and processing speed), performance in these three domains were not considered separately, but instead used to create an aggregate measure of general cognitive function. As Kang et al. rightly note, there is considerable validity and appeal to such an approach [23]. However, in light of recent evidence that the relationship between oral health and cognition may vary across different cognitive domains [12], it was considered important to assess specific cognitive domains separately in the current study.

The current data found that a six-year decline in memory was associated with lower sub-lingual resting saliva pH levels four years later, even following adjustment for an extensive array of potential covariates. Because salivary pH plays a critical role in the maintenance of good oral health, this finding is potentially important. Changes in levels of salivary pH are observed to directly affect the process of tooth remineralization in response to an acid exposure/attack in the oral cavity. Evidence also indicates that prolonged exposure to acidic saliva (pH < 6.6) leads to demineralization of teeth [44]. The inability to maintain one’s own oral care due to increasing physical and cognitive impairment results in very poor oral hygiene and an increase in aciduric pathogenic biofilm throughout the mouth. The combination of poor oral hygiene and poor saliva quality with lower pH often leads to multiple rapidly progressing decaying teeth, some with pulpal exposures causing infections in bone that add to the inflammatory bioburden of the individual. Indeed, it has recently been argued that salivary pH should be regarded as a “quick, chairside diagnostic, biomarker of oral health status” [45].

While no prior study involving older adults has tested whether cognitive decline predicts later salivary pH specifically, recent studies have identified links between age-related cognitive decline and other aspects of salivary gland function. Sorensen et al. [46] identified hyposalivation as a correlate of age-related cognitive decline in middle-aged males, while Do et al. [47] showed that salivary flow rate was independently associated with cognitive impairment among older adults, even after controlling for various confounders. It has been suggested that the central autonomic control pathways that regulate salivary gland function might be affected by the same degenerative processes that contribute to reduced cognitive function [46]. One of the central ways in which salivary gland dysfunction may present is via disturbances in the salivary buffer system, which is responsible for maintaining salivary pH at a relatively constant level. Because salivary gland hypofunction is associated with low pH conditions in the oral environment [48], decline in higher level brain systems might therefore also present as lower pH. Future work is now needed to test this possibility. However, cognitive decline has also been linked to altered patterns of appetite and eating disturbances [49]. Because these altered patterns of eating often include an increase in cravings for unhealthy foods [50], this might also explain why cognitive status is related to lower pH, as diet quality has been shown to be an important determinant of salivary pH [51]. Thus, there are a number of potential biological mechanisms that might explain why a relationship between cognitive decline and salivary pH exists.

Finally, the current study had various strengths, including a longitudinal design, a well-characterized cohort of older adults, and the ability to control for several confounding factors. In addition, a comprehensive neurocognitive battery allowed detailed assessment of different cognitive domains and the use of multiple tests within each cognitive domain minimised the potential influence of test-specific factors. In addition, this study used multiple well validated tools (OHAT) and objective oral health indicators (e.g., SRSpH, the number of occluding pairs), rather than using subjective indicators (self-report) of oral health.

These positive features noted, it is important to acknowledge the reasons why most of the relationships assessed were not significant. One reason could be attributed to the relatively short four-year follow-up period; it would seem likely that more extended delays between baseline and follow up would have greater sensitivity to any effects of cognitive decline on subsequent oral health. However, this relatively short follow up also makes the finding of a significant association between level of memory decline with an objective indicator of oral health status more striking, and as noted, it is hoped that this specific finding (particularly in light of converging evidence from a large longitudinal study focused on middle-aged adults) will encourage further assessment of this specific indicator in understanding how oral health and cognitive function might be related. Another factor which could have meant that the relationship between cognitive function and oral health was underestimated in the current study was that the older individuals sampled were generally healthy and reported good levels of oral health. Further, limitations of the present study include the absence of detailed neuropsychological data at Wave 6 and oral health information at baseline. We also do not know if their oral health was good or poor at baseline. This clearly limits any temporal inferences that can be made, and to provide a direct test of direction of temporality, future research is needed that incorporates baseline oral health data. Moreover, future studies are now also required to understand whether this association between salivary pH and memory is robust, and if so, the underlying mechanism. If so, these findings could have implications for policy makers and health care providers to help develop and implement targeted interventions to improve oral health outcomes and quality of life in older adults with deteriorating memory function.

Conclusion

To conclude, the key finding to emerge from this longitudinal cohort study involving 339 older adults was that memory decline over a six-year period (but no other aspect of cognitive function) predicted lower sub-lingual resting salivary pH four years later. Because lower salivary pH is considered to be an objective indicator of oral health, these data provide further important insights into our understanding of exactly when and how oral health and cognitive function may be linked in late adulthood. However, the findings of the present study must be interpreted cautiously given the cross-sectional nature of oral health data. In particular, findings from this study suggests that older adults who present with greater than normal memory decline may also be at risk of poorer oral health.

Availability of data and materials

The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy or ethical restrictions.

Abbreviations

MAS:

Memory and ageing study

MMSE:

Mini mental state examination

OHAT:

Oral health assessment tool

SRSpH:

Sub-lingual resting salivary pH

RAVLT:

Rey auditory visual verbal learning test

BVRT:

Benton visual retention test

SD:

Standard deviation

GDS:

Geriatric depression scale

B-ADL:

Bayer activities of daily living

ADL:

Activities of daily living

References

  1. United Nations. World population ageing 2019: highlights: report; United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division: New York, 2019.

  2. Suzman R, Beard JR, Boerma T, Chatterji S. Health in an ageing world-what do we know? The Lancet. 2015;385:484–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. 2020;396:413–46.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Petersen PE. The World Oral Health Report 2003: continuous improvement of oral health in the 21st century–the approach of the WHO Global Oral Health Programme. Community Dent Oral Epidemiol. 2003;31:3–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Azzolino D, Passarelli PC, De Angelis P, Piccirillo GB, D’Addona A, Cesari M. Poor oral health as a determinant of malnutrition and sarcopenia. Nutrients. 2019;11:2898.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Tsakos G, et al. Social relationships and oral health among adults aged 60 years or older. Psychosom Med. 2013;75:178–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Sanz M, Del Castillo AM, Jepsen S, Gonzalez-Juanatey JR, D’Aiuto F, Bouchard P, et al. Periodontitis and cardiovascular diseases. Consensus Report Glob Heart. 2020;15:1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Kanagasingam S, Chukkapalli SS, Welbury R, Singhrao SK. Porphyromonas gingivalis is a strong risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis Rep. 2020;4:501–11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Thomson WM, Ma S. An ageing population poses dental challenges. Singapore Dent J. 2014;35:3–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Petersen PE. Challenges to improvement of oral health in the 21st century–the approach of the WHO Global Oral Health Programme. Int Dent J. 2004;54:329–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Wu B, Fillenbaum GG, Plassman BL, Guo L. Association between oral health and cognitive status: a systematic review. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2016;64:739–51.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Nangle MR, Riches J, Grainger SA, Manchery N, Sachdev PS, Henry JD. Oral health and cognitive function in older adults: a systematic review. Gerontology. 2019;65:659–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Nascimento PC, Castro MML, Magno MB, Almeida A, Fagundes N, Maia LC, et al. Association between periodontitis and cognitive impairment in adults: a systematic review. Front Neurol. 2019;10:323.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Nangle MR, Manchery N. Can chronic oral inflammation and masticatory dysfunction contribute to cognitive impairment? Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2020;33:156–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Wu B, Plassman BL, Liang J, Wei L. Cognitive function and dental care utilization among community-dwelling older adults. Am J Public Health. 2007;97:2216–21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Lee KH, Plassman BL, Pan W, Wu B. Mediation effect of oral hygiene on the relationship between cognitive function and oral health in older adults. J Gerontol Nurs. 2016;42:30–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Chen X, Clark JJ, Chen H, Naorungroj S. Cognitive impairment, oral self-care function and dental caries severity in community-dwelling older adults. Gerodontology. 2015;32:53–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Chalmers JM, Carter KD, Spencer AJ. Caries incidence and increments in community-living older adults with and without dementia. Gerodontology. 2002;19:80–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Ship JA, Puckett SA. Longitudinal study on oral health in subjects with Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1994;42:57–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Chen X, Shuman SK, Hodges JS, Gatewood LC, Xu J. Patterns of tooth loss in older adults with and without dementia: a retrospective study based on a Minnesota cohort. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2010;58:2300–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Jones JA, Lavallee N, Alman J, Sinclair C, Garcia RI. Caries incidence in patients with dementia. Gerodontology. 1993;10:76–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Ellefsen B, Holm-Pedersen P, Morse DE, Schroll M, Andersen BB, Waldemar G. Assessing caries increments in elderly patients with and without dementia: a one-year follow-up study. J Am Dent Assoc. 2009;140:1392–400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Kang J, Wu B, Bunce D, Ide M, Pavitt S, Wu J. Cognitive function and oral health among ageing adults. Community Dent Oral Epidemiol. 2019;47:259–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Pitiphat W, Garcia RI, Douglass CW, Joshipura KJ. Validation of self-reported oral health measures. J Public Health Dent. 2002;62:122–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Ramos RQ, Bastos JL, Peres MA. Diagnostic validity of self-reported oral health outcomes in population surveys: literature review. Rev Bras Epidemiol. 2013;16:716–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Sachdev PS, Brodaty H, Reppermund S, Kochan NA, Trollor JN, Draper B, et al. The Sydney Memory and Ageing Study (MAS): methodology and baseline medical and neuropsychiatric characteristics of an elderly epidemiological non-demented cohort of Australians aged 70–90 years. Int Psychogeriatr. 2010;22:1248–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Chalmers JM, King PL, Spencer AJ, Wright FA, Carter KD. The oral health assessment tool-validity and reliability. Aust Dent J. 2005;50:191–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Deutsch A, Siegel E, Cations M, Wright C, Naganathan V, Brodaty H. A pilot study on the feasibility of training nurses to formulate multicomponent oral health interventions in a residential aged care facility. Gerodontology. 2017;34:469–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Lalloo R, Tadakamadla SK, Kroon J, Tut O, Kularatna S, Boase R, et al. Salivary characteristics and dental caries experience in remote Indigenous children in Australia: a cross-sectional study. BMC Oral Health. 2019;19:21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Baliga S, Muglikar S, Kale R. Salivary pH: A diagnostic biomarker. J Indian Soc Periodontol. 2013;17:461–5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Gotfredsen K, Walls AW. What dentition assures oral function? Clin Oral Implants Res. 2007;18:34–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Wechsler D. Wechsler memory scale. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Strauss E, Sherman EMS, Spreen O. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration, norms, and commentary. New York: Oxford University Press; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Benton AL, Sivan AB, Spreen O. Der benton test. Berne: Huber; 1966.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Benton AL. Problems of test construction in the field of aphasia. Cortex. 1967;3:32–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Kaplan E, Goodglass H, Weintraub S. The Boston naming test. Philadelphia: Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Spreen O, Benton AL, University of V, Neuropsychology L. Neurosensory center comprehensive examination for aphasia (NCCEA), 977 revision manual of instructions. Neuropsychology Laboratory, University of Victoria, Victoria; 1977.

  38. Wechsler D. WAIS-R: manual: Wechsler adult intelligence scale–revised. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich [for] Psychological Corp; 1981.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Paradise M, Seruga A, Crawford JD, Chaganti J, Thalamuthu A, Kochan NA, et al. The relationship of cerebral microbleeds to cognition and incident dementia in non-demented older individuals. Brain Imaging Behav. 2019;13:750–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Lipnicki DM, Crawford J, Kochan NA, Trollor JN, Draper B, Reppermund S, et al. Risk factors for mild cognitive impairment, dementia and mortality: The Sydney memory and ageing Study. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2017;18:388–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, Lum O, Huang V, Adey M, et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res. 1982;17:37–49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Hindmarch I, Lehfeld H, de Jongh P, Erzigkeit H. The bayer activities of daily living scale (B-ADL). Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 1998;9:20–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist. 1969;9:179–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Hans R, Thomas S, Garla B, Dagli RJ, Hans MK. Effect of various sugary beverages on salivary pH, flow rate, and oral clearance rate amongst adults. Scientifica (Cairo). 2016;2016:5027283.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Manikandan S, Ayyappan, IIanchezhian T, Saimahitha V. Salivary pH as a diagnostic marker of oral health status. Int J Pharm Sci Rev Res. 2018;51:41-4.

  46. Sorensen CE, Hansen NL, Mortensen EL, Lauritzen M, Osler M, Pedersen AML. Hyposalivation and poor dental health status are potential correlates of age-related cognitive decline in late midlife in Danish men. Front Aging Neurosci. 2018;10:10.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Do MT, Vu H, Lee JK, Park SM, Son JS, Kim H-D. Salivary flow rate and the risk of cognitive impairment among Korean elders: a cross-sectional study. BMC Geriatr. 2021;21(1):245.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Lynge Pedersen AM, Belstrom D. The role of natural salivary defences in maintaining a healthy oral microbiota. J Dent. 2019;80:S3–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Nifli AP. Appetite, metabolism and hormonal regulation in normal ageing and dementia. Diseases. 2018;6:66.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Mungas D, Cooper JK, Weiler PG, Gietzen D, Franzi C, Bernick C. Dietary preference for sweet foods in patients with dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1990;38:999–1007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. de Muniz BR, Maresca BM, Tumilasci OR, Perec CJ. Effects of an experimental diet on parotid saliva and dental plaque pH in institutionalized children. Arch Oral Biol. 1983;28(7):575–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank the participants and their informants for their time and generosity in contributing to this research. We also acknowledge the MAS research team: https://cheba.unsw.edu.au/research-projects/sydney-memory-and-ageing-study, and in particular and study coordinator, Dr. Katya Numbers.

Funding

The Sydney Memory and Ageing Study has been funded by three National Health & Medical Research Council (NHMRC) Program Grants (ID No. ID350833, ID568969, and APP1093083).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors have made substantial contributions to conception and design of the study. NM formulated the research question, interpreted the data, and wrote the manuscript. BCPL assisted with data analysis and interpretation. MRN & JDH assisted with formulating the research question, interpreting the results, and writing the manuscript. NAK, AD, HB & PSS were responsible for acquisition of the data and provided feedback on the study design and manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nithin Manchery.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The Sydney MAS was approved by The University of New South Wales Human Research Ethics Committee (approval number # HC14327), and this particular study by The University of Queensland Human Research Ethics Committee (approval number #2017000770). We confirmed that all methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guideline and regulations. Informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Table S1.

Z-scores for global cognitive function and five cognitive domains. Table S2. Additional analyses estimating effect of cognitive function on Oral Health Assessment Tool (OHAT) score controlling for different covariates. Figure S1. Directed acyclic graphs (DAG) informing casual relationships between cognition and oral health. Table S3. Distribution of oral health variables based on OHAT score.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Manchery, N., Henry, J.D., Lam, B.C.P. et al. Memory decline in older individuals predicts an objective indicator of oral health: findings from the Sydney Memory and Ageing Study. BMC Oral Health 22, 93 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12903-022-02128-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12903-022-02128-y

Keywords